29 and 30 In general, athletes exhibit differences in perceptual-

29 and 30 In general, athletes exhibit differences in perceptual-cognitive abilities when compared to non-athletes. For example, gymnasts outperformed non-athletes in mental rotation task and in general better for pictures of human figures http://www.selleckchem.com/products/Trichostatin-A.html than for pictures of cubed figures,31 suggesting variants of different mental rotation tasks should be applied in testing athletes, since they may have different outcomes

depending on athletes’ type of sport and/or the type of sport that is reflected in the mental rotation stimuli. Although mental rotation is developed at early stage during neuronal development period and the differences of mental rotation between athletes and non-athletes might be related to the subjects with better spatial ability, studies showed that the mental rotation is trainable for better. Therefore, this would be beneficial for our understanding of motor learning based on mental simulation and could contribute to the training of athletes from sports such as gymnastics, soccer, golf, and

more for skydiving, scuba-diving, and climbing, where losses of spatial orientation can be life-threatening.32 Exercises also have positive impact on mental rotation. A study of juggling training showed that 3 months of juggling training improved performance on a chronometric mental rotation task with cube figures, compared to a control group Obeticholic Acid ic50 which did not receive any training.33 Navigation tests, also called a way-finding, Rolziracetam are commonly conducted by having subjects reconstruct a path through a map or real space. There are two different approaches that may be involved: egocentric and allocentric strategies (Fig. 2). An egocentric strategy involves more local landmarks and directional cues as personal directions. An allocentric strategy uses the absolute position of general landmarks, such as distance,

as absolute directions.34 Individuals with hippocampal sclerosis were more impaired in navigating through a virtual maze in which learning was associated with egocentric memory. The allocentric memory impairment is found in patients with extensive hippocampal sclerosis plus subcortical deterioration, suggesting a combination of hippocampal and cortical damage is associated with negative changes in allocentric memory.35 Patients with temporal lobe epilepsy without hippocampal sclerosis do not display cognitive deficits of allocentric or egocentric memory. Although males perform better than females in the navigation strategy, the relationship between navigation and one’s level of testosterone has not been consistently demonstrated.36 and 37 It is known that men tend to favor a more allocentric strategy (accurate judgments of distance), while women are more frequently egocentric (able to recall more street names and building shapes as landmarks) navigators.

, 1987) We used a bootstrap to confirm that neither the bias in

, 1987). We used a bootstrap to confirm that neither the bias in the preferred orientations of the F− cells nor possible spatial clustering of F+ cells artifactually caused the statistical difference. On each bootstrap repetition, for each sharply selective F+ cell, a sharply selective F− cell was chosen randomly within a 50 μm radius. In this way, we obtained a set of randomly chosen sharply selective F− cells that were matched in number and spatial location to the sharply selective F+ cells. The distribution of these

LY2157299 randomly chosen F− cells was compared with the entire set of F− cells with a Kuiper’s test across a wide range of p values. Any significant difference is a false positive. We simulated 100,000 repetitions for each p value and for each clone, and false-positive rates were obtained. We compared the differences in preferred orientations (ΔOri) of all the possible pairs among F+ cells and the ΔOri of all the possible pairs between F+ and F− cells. Again, a bootstrap was used to correct the p values obtained from the Kolmogorov-Smirnov Selleck trans-isomer test. We obtained a set of randomly chosen sharply selective F− cells, matched in number and spatial location to the sharply selective F+ cells, in the same way as described above.

By comparing differences of ΔOri among this population and ΔOri between this population and the entire F− population using a Kolmogorov-Smirnov test, false-positive rates were obtained across a wide range of p values. The p value was corrected with the false-positive rate obtained from the bootstrap at a p value threshold obtained by comparing actual clonally related and unrelated pairs. We thank Dr. R. Clay Reid for his support and discussion and Dr. Toshihiko

Hosoya and Dr. Satoru Kondo for discussion. We thank Garrett Banks for technical support. We appreciate the technical support from the Research Support Center of the Graduate School of Medical Sciences at Kyushu University. This work was supported by grants from CREST-JST, the Takeda Science Foundation, the Uehara Foundation and Kowa Foundation (to K.O.), and the David and Lucille Packard Foundation (to C.L). “
“Altruistic acts involve costs for the actor and benefits for another individual. Altruism in most animal Rutecarpine species is directed toward genetically related individuals (Hamilton, 1964). In contrast, human altruism goes far beyond helping kin. A significant number of people help strangers and reciprocate favors even when they do not know their interaction partners and will never meet them again (Camerer, 2003 and Henrich et al., 2005). Human history has repeatedly shown that some people are even willing to risk their lives in order to contribute to some of the most important public goods—democracy and liberty. However, there is also enormous individual heterogeneity in human altruism, and the sources of individual variation are still very poorly understood.

, 2003) The organization found by Bathellier et al (2012)—which

, 2003). The organization found by Bathellier et al. (2012)—which contained a small number of discrete, spatially separated modes—could thus be a more natural behavior for locally recurrent circuits that are not fine tuned. Like all surprising results, this study raises many questions. First, how general is this organization of neuronal activity? Evidence for attractor dynamics in other networks has been inconsistent (e.g., Wills et al., 2005; Leutgeb et al., 2005). Is a similar organization of discrete modes Fludarabine molecular weight seen in other cortical regions, other cortical layers,

and other brain structures? Second, would a similar pattern be seen in actively behaving animals, as well as the anesthetized and awake passive mice studied here? Third, what causes particular groups of cells to form an assembly? In Hebb’s original theory, the composition of cell assemblies was determined by experience. But Bathellier et al. (2012) could predict one mouse’s classification choices from the mode organization observed in different animals, suggesting that auditory cortical assemblies arise either from an innate process, or at least from commonalities in the sensory experience of these mice. Finally, why should the cortex work like this, using hundreds of neurons

do convey a single number? Although this may seem inefficient from the perspective of information coding, the brain is not just there to represent external stimuli, but to act on them. Is cortical attractor dynamics in fact a fundamental mechanism

of decision making? Characterizing cortical dynamics in behaving animals, and how it changes Androgen Receptor phosphorylation with learning, may well answer these questions. “
“Charles Darwin famously wrote that the eye caused him to doubt that random selection could create the intricacies of nature. Fortunately, Darwin did not know the structure of the else retina: if he had, his slowly gestating treatise on evolution might never have been published at all. Among other wonders, the neurons of the retina are tiny (Figure 1). The ∼100 million rod photoreceptors appear to be the second most numerous neurons of the human body, after only the cerebellar granule cells. The retina’s projection neuron, the retinal ganglion cell, has less than 1% the soma-dendritic volume of a cortical or hippocampal pyramidal cell. Although the retina forms a sheet of tissue only ∼200 μm thick, its neural networks carry out feats of image processing that were unimagined even a few years ago (Gollisch and Meister, 2010). They require a rethinking not only of the retina’s function, but of the brain mechanisms that shape these signals into behaviorally useful visual perception. The retinal neurome—the census of its component cells—continues to be refined. An initial estimate of 55 cell types in the retina (Masland, 2001) appears to have been something of an underestimate.

, 2013)? What sort of interplay exists between LRRTM4, LRRTM1/2,

, 2013)? What sort of interplay exists between LRRTM4, LRRTM1/2, and TARPs—all of which are expressed in the DG—in AMPAR regulation? Do they compete or cooperate with each other? Lastly, copy number variations of LRRTM4 and glypican 6 have been see more associated with autism spectrum disorders (Pinto et al., 2010). In addition, mutant mice that lack Ext1, which encodes an enzyme essential for HS biosynthesis, display autistic-like behaviors including impaired social interaction, reduced ultrasonic vocalizations,

and repetitive behaviors ( Irie et al., 2012). Whether and how deficits in LRRTM4-HSPG interactions contribute to the development of these disorders remain open questions. In conclusion, the two new studies demonstrate that postsynaptic LRRTM4 trans-synaptically interacts with presynaptic HSPGs to promote excitatory synapse development, identifying HSPGs as an unexpected group of presynaptic organizers. Fruitful avenues for future research include determining whether similar HSPG-dependent trans-synaptic adhesions are widespread

in various brain regions. If so, this would reinforce the importance of this newly emerging group of presynaptic organizers. “
“In selleck screening library the 1964 film Mary Poppins, Julie Andrews touts how “a spoonful of sugar helps the medicine go down,” reflecting that addition of sweet taste agonists can mask the presence of bitter compounds, like most medicines.

New work from Craig Montell’s laboratory studying taste behavior in Drosophila melanogastor ( Jeong et al., 2013) reveals how flies would not be easily swayed by Mary Poppins into taking their medicine by mixing bitter with sugar. Flies have distinct taste neurons tuned to bitter or sweet compounds, but when sweet and bitter compounds are mixed, the bitter tastants turn off the drive to consume sugar. In this issue of Neuron, Jeong et al. (2013) Olopatadine show the surprising mechanism behind how bitters trump sweet, through association with the odorant-binding protein (OBP) OBP49a and suppression of the sweet neuron activity ( Jeong et al., 2013). Taste is a critical sense that allows animals to evaluate the quality of food sources. Sweet taste is associated with the presence of energy-rich sugars, while bitter taste is associated with toxic or noxious compounds that might threaten the health of the animal. Sweet and bitter tastes are mediated by membrane receptors expressed on taste neurons that specifically detect these compounds. Receptors detecting sweet compounds are expressed by different neurons than those detecting bitter compounds, establishing “labeled lines” that allow the brain to distinguish between good, energy-rich foods and potentially toxic ones. There are approximately 120 taste neurons located in sensilla on the labellum (mouth) of the fly (Figures 1A and 1B).

, 2008), and analyzed CTGF expression 4, 8, 12, and 20 days posti

, 2008), and analyzed CTGF expression 4, 8, 12, and 20 days postinjection (Figure 6H). Ablation of olfactory receptor neurons was confirmed by impairment in food search of olfactotoxin-injected mice

(Figures S6A and S6B) and by decreased expression of the olfactory marker protein (OMP) (Figures S6C and S6D). Suppression of the sensory input led to a significant decrease in CTGF expression already 4 days postinjection, and at 12 days postinjection CTGF expression was barely detectable (Figure 6I and Figure S6E for quantification). CTGF expression was partially restored 20 days postinjection, correlating with partial reinnervation of olfactory sensory input as reported (Alonso et al., 2008). Decreased CTGF expression levels following olfactotoxin treatment did not result from a demise of external tufted cells (i.e., CTGF-expressing cells), as was evidenced by an unchanged number of CCK-positive cells (data not shown). Conversely, Cabozantinib concentration TGF-β2 expression was strongly elevated during the period of olfactory input impairment and reached a maximum 8 days

postinjection (Figure 6J). Ctgf is an immediate-early gene with a short half-life ( Kroening et al., 2009) and is thus suited to test whether environmental changes at a fast timescale alter CTGF expression levels. The first indication for the activity-dependent regulation of CTGF expression derives from its coexpression with c-fos, another activity-dependent immediate-early gene ( Figure S6F). Since suppression of olfactory input resulted in a reduction of CTGF expression, we hypothesized that olfactory enrichment augments CTGF expression. Mice were presented daily selleck chemical Cell press with three distinct odors for three weeks (63 distinct odorants in total) ( Figure S6G), and CTGF expression levels were evaluated thereafter in 200 glomeruli ( Figures S6H and S6I). Glomeruli were subdivided according to their intensity of CTGF expression into four categories: very high (>10 units), high (5–10 units), intermediate (1–5 units), and low (0–1 units) (for details, see the Supplemental

Experimental Procedures). There was no difference in CTGF expression levels between controls and mice subjected to an odor-enriched environment ( Figure S6J). This is not too surprising, as one would expect to see a potential augmentation of the short-lived CTGF only in few glomeruli that were activated by the last few odorants. We also measured whether the enrichment paradigm resulted in changes of neuronal survival in the glomerular layer (see BrdU treatment rationale in Figure S6G) and found no difference in BrdU-positive cell number between control and odor-enriched mice ( Figure S6K). To investigate the possible activity dependence of CTGF expression in specific glomeruli, we employed transgenic mice, namely MOR23-IRES-tauGFP, in which only two glomeruli per OB are EGFP labeled and hence can be surveyed over time (Vassalli et al., 2002).

Thus, the output from late-bursting cells is principally determin

Thus, the output from late-bursting cells is principally determined by mGluR activation, which always

see more leads to enhancement of bursting, while the output from early-bursting cells is regulated by both mGluR, which leads to suppression of bursting on its own, and mAChRs, which lead to enhancement of bursting during coactivation with mGluRs. It remains possible that another condition, not yet discovered, could result in downregulation of bursting in late-bursting cells, thus completing a suite of conditions that lead to bidirectional modulation of both cell types in the intact brain. Our findings could promote a better understanding of the well-established dichotomy regarding the role of acetylcholine in learning and memory. Decades of work have shown that cholinergic input facilitates hippocampal activity during memory encoding and learning but suppresses activity during memory retrieval and recall (Drever et al., 2011; Hasselmo, 1999; Micheau and Marighetto, 2011). Our results provide a potential framework for studying the mechanisms of this biphasic role of acetylcholine, as the two types of cells that process and transmit hippocampal information can be differentially modulated by mAChR activation. Furthermore, as projections to CA1 from the entorhinal cortex are more

sensitive to mGluR-dependent presynaptic inhibition than mAChR-dependent inhibition (Giocomo and Hasselmo, 2007), there may be differential modulation of separate information streams flowing directly to CA1 from entorhinal cortex and indirectly through the selleck trisynaptic circuit of the hippocampus. Recent work in vivo has shown that cells with a higher propensity to burst are more likely to become place cells (Epsztein et al., 2010). On the surface, this would suggest that early-bursting cells are more likely to become place cells. As most of the cells in the CA1 region

are late bursting (Jarsky et al., 2008), however, whatever and as place cells are abundant in this region (Moser et al., 2008; Nakazawa et al., 2004; O’Keefe, 1976), it seems unlikely that late-bursting cells are not place cells. Rather, it is possible that both cell types can become place cells and that modulation of neuronal firing patterns with forms of plasticity similar to those described here may serve to enhance or suppress excitability, thus affecting which neurons are likely to exhibit place fields in a particular environment. Similarly, modulation of bursting could contribute to the formation of nonspatial behavioral contingencies on firing (Pastalkova et al., 2008; Wood et al., 2000). ACSF consisted of 125 mM NaCl, 2.5 mM KCl, 25 mM NaHCO3, 1.25 mM NaH2PO4, 1 mM MgCl2, 2 mM CaCl2, and 25 mM dextrose (Fisher Scientific). The pH of the ACSF was 7.3 and the osmolarity was 305–320 mOsm. ACSF was oxygenated and pH buffered by constant bubbling with a gas mixture of 95% O2/5% CO2.

54 (95% CI 0 38 to 0 70, p < 0 001, random effects meta-analysis,

54 (95% CI 0.38 to 0.70, p < 0.001, random effects meta-analysis, I2 = 12%). There was a bigger effect on strength in the trials in which the programs targeted strength specifically (by using weights with a moderate to high intensity, ie, using a weight so heavy that only 8–12 repetitions could

be done without resting). The pooled effect from the 7 programs that did not target strength specifically was 0.32 (95% CI 0.09 to 0.55) whereas the pooled effect from the 10 programs that did specifically target strength was 0.68 (95% CI 0.49 to 0.87). This check details difference was statistically significant (effect of strength in meta-regression, p = 0.045) ( Figure 2). The meta-analysis of balance outcomes included six trials and found a moderate effect of physical activity on balance (SMD = 0.52, 95% CI 0.24 to 0.79, random effects meta-analysis, I2 = 51%) (Figure 3). The meta-analysis of endurance outcomes included six trials (8 comparisons, as one trial had three groups) and found a moderate effect of physical activity on endurance (SMD = 0.73, 95% CI 0.50 to 0.96, p < 0.001, random effects meta-analysis, I2 = 65%) ( Figure 4). Only one trial (Pereira et al 1998) reported on the effects of a physical activity program on long-term falls.

Pereira et al 1998 showed a non-significant decrease in the occurrence of falls over the last 12 months (RR 0.82, 95% CI 0.53 to 1.26). Of those who received a walking program 15 years earlier, 27% percent reported falling in the year prior Liothyronine Sodium to follow-up, whereas 33% of selleck kinase inhibitor the control group reported falling in the past year. The rate of women reporting more than one fall over the last 12 months was also lower in the walking group (23%) when compared to controls (30%) but this difference was not statistically significant (RR 0.76, 95% CI 0.48 to 1.23). Adherence to the physical activity programs, presented in Table 2, was assessed in 12 of the 22 included trials (Asikainen et al 2006, Bemben et al 2000, Heinonen et al 1998, Janzen et al 2006, King et al 1991, Klentrou et al 2007, Levinger et al 2007, Mitchell et

al 1998, Sallinen et al 2007, Shirazi et al 2007, Singh et al 2009, Uusi-Rasi et al 2003). In general, physical activity adherence (calculated as the percentage of completed physical activity hours, out of the prescribed hours) was greater than 80% (Asikainen et al 2006, Bemben et al 2000, Janzen et al 2006, Levinger et al 2007, Mitchell et al 1998, Sallinen et al 2007, Singh et al 2009), ranging from 48% (Shirazi et al 2007) to 96% (Levinger et al 2007). This systematic review found that strength, balance and endurance can clearly be improved by physical activity in people aged 40–65. The effect of physical activity on falls has not been well investigated in this age group. Most of the trials identified focused on strength and/or endurance training. This review found a moderate effect of physical activity on muscle strength.

2c and a), in contrast to what was obtained with NaIO4

(F

2c and a), in contrast to what was obtained with NaIO4

(Fig. 2b). OAg-oxTEMPO Selleckchem Ruxolitinib with an average percentage number of oxidized repeating units of 36% and 15% were conjugated to CRM197, to investigate the impact of the degree of OAg derivatization on the immunogenicity of the corresponding conjugates. The same conditions for the conjugation and purification of OAg-oxNaIO4 were applied and in both cases all CRM197 in the reaction mixtures was conjugated, with 19–28% of OAg conjugated (Fig. 3b). Conjugates obtained using less derivatized OAg (both after treatment with NaIO4 or TEMPO) were characterized by a higher OAg to protein ratio with respect to the conjugate obtained from more oxidized OAg which was able to couple to more CRM197 molecules (Table 1). The terminal KDO residue of the core oligosaccharide was used for selective linking of OAg to CRM197 without modifying the OAg chain. To generate one conjugate vaccine, reductive amination C646 solubility dmso with ADH was followed by reaction with SIDEA and conjugation to CRM197[28]. A similar chemistry was evaluated where the first

step of reductive amination was conducted with NH4OAc, allowing the synthesis of a conjugate with a linker about half the length of ADH-SIDEA (Fig. 1b). After testing the reactivity of OAg-KDO with NH4OAc under different conditions (see SI), in order to synthesize the corresponding conjugate, the reaction was performed at pH 7.0 for 5 days resulting in the activation of 90% of OAg chains. Use of the longer ADH linker with the hydrazide functionality allowed for the reaction to proceed, with activation close to 100% after only 2 h at pH 4.5. In the following step where the OAg derivatives were reacted with SIDEA, >90% of total NH2

groups were coupled to SIDEA, for both OAg-NH2 and OAg-ADH. The analysis of the corresponding conjugation mixtures by HPLC-SEC, confirmed conjugate formation without residual free protein, while the amount of conjugated OAg was close to 15% in both cases. The resulting conjugates were very similar in terms of OAg to CRM197 ratio (4–5 OAg chains linked per protein) and molecular size, measured as distribution coefficient Kd by HPLC-SEC; even if OAg-NH2-SIDEA-CRM197 showed a slightly broader population (Table 1, Fig. 3c). Selective conjugates contained higher OAg to protein ratios than random conjugates (Table 1). The synthesized conjugates were tested in mice, with the following main objectives: to compare the immunogenicity of random versus selective conjugates; to analyze the impact of linker chain length on the immunogenicity of selective conjugates; to evaluate whether the degree of random modification of the OAg chain impacts on immunogenicity. After three doses, all the conjugates generated anti-OAg IgG levels that were not statistically different (Fig. 4a).

, 2009) Boosting mGluR1s, for example with positive allosteric m

, 2009). Boosting mGluR1s, for example with positive allosteric modulators I-BET151 research buy to remove GluN3-containing NMDARs, may ultimately restore normal synaptic transmission, prevent adaptations in downstream circuits, and stop the development of addiction. C57/BL6 mice (male and female)

and NR3A heterozygotes and knockouts were injected with cocaine (15 mg/kg i.p.) or the same volume of saline as controls. The dose of cocaine that we used did not induce seizures or increase mortality. The study was conducted in accordance with the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the University of Geneva and with permission of the cantonal authorities (Permit No. 1007/3592/2). The majority of electrophysiology recordings were undertaken in young mice (P14–40) to facilitate identification of VTA cells. However,

note that maturation of excitatory transmission in the VTA of mice is complete at P14 (Bellone Cabozantinib purchase et al., 2011) and we have also reported cocaine-evoked plasticity of VTA DA neuron excitatory synapses in adult mice (aged 7 months, Mameli et al., 2009). After animals were sacrificed, 250-μm-thick horizontal midbrain slices containing the VTA were prepared and whole-cell voltage-clamp recordings were made as previously shown (Bellone and Lüscher, 2006). The access resistance was monitored by a hyperpolarizing step of −14 mV with each sweep, every 10 s. The cells were recorded at the access resistance from 10–25 MΩ, and data were excluded when the resistance changed > 20%. Synaptic currents were evoked by stimuli (0.05–0.1 ms) at 0.1 Hz through a stimulating electrode placed rostral to the VTA. The experiments were carried out in the presence of GABAA receptor antagonist picrotoxin (100 μM); the AMPAR EPSCs were pharmacologically isolated by application of the NMDARs antagonist D,

L-APV (100 μM), whereas the NMDA EPSCs were pharmacologically isolated by the application of the AMPARs antagonist NBQX (10 μM). Dipeptidyl peptidase Representative example traces are shown as the average of 20 consecutive EPSCs typically obtained at each potential or, in the case of plasticity protocols, during the last 5 min of the baseline and at least 30 min after the induction of plasticity. The decay time τw of NMDA EPSC was calculated as described previously (Bellone and Nicoll, 2007). The rectification index of AMPARs is the ratio of the chord conductance calculated at negative potential divided by the chord conductance at positive potentials. I-V curves of pharmacologically isolated NMDARs were generated holding the cells at different membrane potential for 5 min each and normalizing EPSCs at 40 mV. Tricine (N-tris(hydroxymethyl)methylglycine, 10 mM) was used to buffer zinc following the relationship [Zn]free = [Zn]added/200 ( Paoletti et al., 1997). All drugs were purchased from Tocris, except Tetanus Toxin that was purchased from Sigma Aldrich.

The average of three maximum force measurements was recorded The

The average of three maximum force measurements was recorded. The participants held each contraction for 5 s. Trunk flexion and extension were performed while standing, with the pelvis stabilized,

and without upper extremity support. The attachment was placed two inches below the participant’s sternal notch for trunk flexion (Fig. 1) and between the scapulae for trunk extension (Fig. 2). Bilateral hip extension and abduction force was Gefitinib order collected while standing, with the hips in neutral position, and without upper extremity support. The attachment was placed two inches above the posterior joint line of the knee for hip extension and two inches above the lateral joint line of the knee for abduction. The bilateral hip external Compound C price rotation force was measured in sitting. The participant’s hips and knees were flexed at 90° without upper extremity

support. The attachment was placed two inches above the ankle joint. The isoinertial strength test was a timed sit-up test. The protocol for the sit-up test was developed by the American Alliance of Health, Physical Education, Recreation, and Dance (AAHPERD17). The objective of the test was to perform as many full sit-ups as possible within 1 min. The sit-up test was initiated in the hook-lying position, with arms held across chest, knees flexed at 90°, and feet secured. To complete a full sit-up, the participant’s scapulae touched the mat in the lying position and the elbows made contact with the knees in sitting. Following protocols established by McGill et al.,18 four core endurance tests were performed. The objective of the endurance tests was to hold a static position for as long as possible. The endurance tests were the trunk flexor test, trunk extensor test, and bilateral side bridge tests. The trunk flexor 3-mercaptopyruvate sulfurtransferase test began with the participant

in the sit-up position with their trunk supported at 60° of trunk flexion. Knees and hips were flexed at 90°, arms crossed over chest, and feet secured. The support of the trunk was then removed, and the participant held the position for as long as possible. The test was terminated when the participant was no longer able to hold the position. The trunk extensor test was performed with the participant lying prone on a treatment table. Their pelvis, hips, and knees were secured to the treatment table, while a chair at the same height as the surface of the table supported the trunk and upper extremities. The chair was removed, and the individual held a horizontal body position for as long as possible with arms crossed over chest. The test was discontinued when the participant fell below the horizontal position. The side bridge tests were performed in the side-lying position on a treatment table. The participant’s knees were extended with the top foot placed in front of the lower foot. The participant supported their weight only on their lower elbow and feet while lifting their hips off the mat.